Sentences with phrase «infant is in distress»

Further, when an infant is in distress, has a difficult temperament or is ill, it affects mothers and their ability to adapt to demands, both at work and at home.
If raising mini-humans who are in touch with and can effectively manage emotions is a goal for parents, Dr Sarah Buckley, family physician and author of Gentle Birth, Gentle Mothering, affirms the importance of consistent parental emotional love and support — particularly when our infants are in distress.

Not exact matches

The causes of infant mortality in Bangladesh have been studied and include: Birth asphyxia (45 %), prematurity / low birthweight (15 %), sepsis / meningitis (12 %), respiratory distress syndrome (7 %), and pneumonia (6 %) were the major direct causes of death (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2965329/).
If we place infants in playpens and cribs and don't co-sleep, we may miss the early cues that babies are in distress.
Although infants only cry to tell parents their need, their cries may be different according to the distress they are in.
In spite of our vigilance, preterm births are on the rise, cerebral palsy - thought to be caused by fetal distress - rates have remained stagnant, and in 2002, infant mortality rose for the first time since 195In spite of our vigilance, preterm births are on the rise, cerebral palsy - thought to be caused by fetal distress - rates have remained stagnant, and in 2002, infant mortality rose for the first time since 195in 2002, infant mortality rose for the first time since 1958.
For example, it was discovered that in Western cultures, mothers responded to only 30 — 50 % of their infants» babbling and 50 — 75 % of their expressions of distress.
If you have other children to care for or need to work to pay for medical bills, then NOT being in the NICU 24/7 feeling helpless and being distressed looking at your tiny, sick infant may be the best thing you can do for your baby and yourself.
One commonly seen complication in premature babies is Infant Respiratory Distress Syndrome (RDS).
The immune system of people who are either celiac positive or gluten sensitive will perceive this protein as a foreign invader and thus cause a reaction which can lead to a variety of signs and symptoms: • Abdominal distress, gas, bloating, chronic diarrhea, nausea • Fatigue • Joint pain • ADHD / ADD and behavioral issues • Delayed growth or failure to thrive in infants • Seizures • Acid reflux • Headaches Gluten sensitivity should be considered as an underlying cause when dealing with a chronic condition.
The same is going to happen with CIO when research proves how damaging it is to leave infants crying alone in distress.
Distress escalates to rage, but then turns to a loss of hope, often culminating in the development of dissociative states or self - defeating aggression, or a sense of despair seen in the biological shutdown which is seen in failure - to - thrive infants.
When the baby is greatly distressed, it creates conditions for damge to synapses, the network construction which is ongoing in the infant brain.
Excessive infant crying in this setting may be only a «red flag» of distress, and as such reflects key co-existent elements of the context in which it occurs, such as the role of the father, social and economic factors and the community context.
In a study of Dutch infants, co-sleeping babies experienced lower spikes in the stress hormone cortisol when they were subjected to psychologically distressing situations (Beijers et al 2013In a study of Dutch infants, co-sleeping babies experienced lower spikes in the stress hormone cortisol when they were subjected to psychologically distressing situations (Beijers et al 2013in the stress hormone cortisol when they were subjected to psychologically distressing situations (Beijers et al 2013).
Another perspective for improving maternal care was offered by the group of Young and colleges who tested the potential contribution of musical training in depressed adults to their ability to interpret infant crying in relation to changes in pitch.51 Using auditory recordings of infants crying manipulated such that the pitch of the crying increased gradually to sound more distressed, it was shown that depressed adults with previous musical training showed higher sensitivity in discriminating distress variations in the infant crying.
Although this study was not specifically tested on mothers with PPD, Young suggests that even short musical training can have a protective effect to overcome diminished sensitivity to auditory cues for distress in infant crying that mothers with PDD might have.
Depressed mothers gaze less at their infants, rock less, are less active and show poorer responsiveness to their infants.32 Infants may be more drowsy, more distressed and fussy, look less at their mothers and engage in more self - directed actiinfants, rock less, are less active and show poorer responsiveness to their infants.32 Infants may be more drowsy, more distressed and fussy, look less at their mothers and engage in more self - directed actiinfants.32 Infants may be more drowsy, more distressed and fussy, look less at their mothers and engage in more self - directed actiInfants may be more drowsy, more distressed and fussy, look less at their mothers and engage in more self - directed activity.33
Whether in pain, or experiencing some other distress, infants who are crying need assistance to reduce their distress, return their stress response system and the functioning of the HPA axis to a calm state, and train up their vagal nerve for healthful functioning.
Helping parents focus on steps for keeping infants calm and soothing infants when they are upset, as well as remaining with infants even if they continue to cry, may be an important step in reducing instances of infant distress.
Getting infants to sleep is one of the most common (and distressing) problems parents face, and it is very significant in the manner it influences parents.
In addition, mothers with negatively temperamental infants were more likely to show maternal distress and less social support from prenatal to 12 weeks postpartum.
While infants and babies (and children in distress) should be responded to promptly, I believe it's alright for us to ask our children to wait a bit for our attention at times.
Mothers reported more symptoms of psychological distress24, 25 and low self - efficacy.26, 27 And, although mothers report more depressive symptoms at the time their infants are experiencing colic, 28,29 research on maternal depression 3 months after the remittance of infant colic is mixed.30, 31 The distress mothers of colic infants report may arise out of their difficulties in soothing their infants as well as within their everyday dyadic interactions.32 The few studies to date that have examined the long - term consequences of having a colicky child, however, indicate that there are no negative outcomes for parent behaviour and, importantly, for the parent - child relationship.
The psychosocial outcome receiving the most attention from researchers is problem behaviour, with most studies finding perceived negative reactivity in infancy to predict problem behaviour in childhood33, 34 and adolescent.35 Specifically, infants prone to high levels of fear, frustration, and sadness, as well as difficulty recovering from such distress, were found to be at increased risk for internalizing and externalizing problem behaviours according to parental and / or teacher report.
For example, four - month - olds who show high levels of motor activity and distress, called high - reactive, are likely to become inhibited to the unfamiliar at 1 - 2 years old and report more unrealistic worries and more frequent bouts of depression at age 18, whereas low - reactive infants are likely to become uninhibited to the unfamiliar in the second year and are at a slightly higher risk for asocial behaviour at age 18.
Whether an infant is born prematurely, has a congenital anomaly, or is coping with infection, respiratory distress, or another concern, we do everything in our power to ensure that your baby not only recovers, but also thrives.
We do cesareans all the time for babies we perceive to be in danger, even though the vast majority of those infants would have delivered fine if we had been blissfully ignorant of their apparent distress.
Although the Australian work of McIntosh (2010) found that infants under two who spent one night or more a week and toddlers who spend 10 days a month of overnight time in their non-primary caregiver's care are more irritable, more severely distressed and insecure in their relationships with their primary parent, less persistent at tasks, and more physically and emotionally stressed, this study has been largely discredited by a recently published consensus report endorsed by 110 child development experts (Warshak, 2013), which found that McIntosh drew unwarranted conclusions from her unrepresentative and flawed data.
Fast forward one year, on call another night, and we were transferred an infant in respiratory distress following a home water birth.
because the stress hormones that are released from distressed crying, destroy nerve connections in critical portions of an infant's developing brain and if repeated, can alter their brain structure and responses to stress into adulthood.
Mothers who breastfeed have been found to report lower levels of perceived stress and negative mood, higher levels of maternal attachment, and tend to perceive their infants more positively than mothers who formula - feed.9, 19 - 21 There is evidence to suggest that breastfeeding mothers may also spend more time in emotional care and be more sensitive to infant emotional distress cues than bottle - feeding mothers.22, 23 Relatedly, a small fMRI study of 17 mothers in the first postpartum month, found that breastfeeding mothers showed greater activation in brain areas involved in empathy and bonding than formula - feeding mothers when listening to their own infant's cry.24 These brain areas included the superior frontal gyrus, insula, precuneus, striatum and amygdala.
The report theorizes that, by having the infant in the room with you, you are better able to respond to any distress or sign that something is «off.»
«We found that babies exposed to opioids pain relievers were more likely to be born preterm, have complicated births, low birth weight and have complications such as meconium aspiration syndrome (a sign of infant distress at birth) and respiratory distress,» said lead author Stephen Patrick, M.D., MPH, assistant professor of Pediatrics and Health Policy in the Division of Neonatology with the Monroe Carell Jr..
The study showed that children who had been more distressed as infants and had received less physical contact had a molecular profile in their cells that was underdeveloped for their age — pointing to the possibility that they were lagging biologically.
According to the plaintiff, during the latter part of the pregnancy and on the day the baby was born, the defendants failed to respond properly to obvious signs of fetal distress which caused serious hypoxia; this resulted in the infant suffering permanent brain damage.
Parenting skills interventions that focus on these factors provide anticipatory guidance and teach parents how to identify and respond appropriately to infant cues and distress to positively influence self - regulatory capacities, well - being and the developing control of the infant's food intake in order to avoid eating in the absence of hunger have shown beneficial results.84 85 92 93
For example, four - month - olds who show high levels of motor activity and distress, called high - reactive, are likely to become inhibited to the unfamiliar at 1 - 2 years old and report more unrealistic worries and more frequent bouts of depression at age 18, whereas low - reactive infants are likely to become uninhibited to the unfamiliar in the second year and are at a slightly higher risk for asocial behaviour at age 18.
Although infants are able to recognize their parents by voice or smell within the first weeks of life, they accept care from any caregiver during this phase without distress or anxiety (Lamb et al., in press).
Evidence suggests that, in the first six months, infants are capable of experiencing and responding to distress by adopting self - soothing behavior such as sucking.
The great individual variation in these capabilities can be described by dimensions such as the infant's disposition for distress or negative emotionality, irritability and soothability [57, 58].
Looks at the responses of preschool children to classmates in distress in relation to «attachment» profiles of those children when they were infants.
As infant distress increased in duration, mothers with BPD were increasingly likely to show insensitive behaviour towards their child.
Two major reasons for this view are (1) the strong similarities between monkeys and humans in social behavior, endocrine function, brain structure, and degree and duration of mother - infant nurturance (Harlow and Zimmerman 1959; Kalin and Shelton 2003; Mendoza and Mason 1997), or, in the unique case of titi monkeys, the extent of biparental care (Hennessy 1997); and (2) the extent to which monkeys fulfill Ainsworth's criteria of attachment (Ainsworth 1972), namely, unequivocal distress upon complete separation from the attachment figure and alleviation of this distress (both behavioral and physiological) upon reunion / interaction with the attachment figure (Mendoza and Mason 1997).
In contrast, if the caregiver has been unavailable or only erratically available or insensitive or rejecting when the infant has sought contact, the infant will learn not to seek contact when distressed or to seek comfort only in an ambivalent manner, as strong bids might alienate an already unreliable caregiveIn contrast, if the caregiver has been unavailable or only erratically available or insensitive or rejecting when the infant has sought contact, the infant will learn not to seek contact when distressed or to seek comfort only in an ambivalent manner, as strong bids might alienate an already unreliable caregivein an ambivalent manner, as strong bids might alienate an already unreliable caregiver.
Infant Mental Health Journal, Volume 20, Issue 1, Pages 77 - 86 (Seventy - two low - income mothers and their 15 - month - old infants were evaluated at home and in the laboratory to determine whether mothers» reports of distress and partner violence were associated with infant - mother attachment and infant mastery motivInfant Mental Health Journal, Volume 20, Issue 1, Pages 77 - 86 (Seventy - two low - income mothers and their 15 - month - old infants were evaluated at home and in the laboratory to determine whether mothers» reports of distress and partner violence were associated with infant - mother attachment and infant mastery motivinfant - mother attachment and infant mastery motivinfant mastery motivation.
These questions concern (1) how changes in cortisol are associated with hypothesized sex - specific stressors of out - group threat for males and infant distress in females, (2) how exposure to sex - specific stressors are related to the desire of affiliation, and (3) how changes in cortisol are associated with mortality salience induction.
Increased cortisol responses were predicted in men exposed to an out - group stressor (inflammatory speech / funeral protest) following MS induction and in females exposed to cues of infant distress (i.e., crying).
Specifically, the following questions were addressed: (1) Are changes in cortisol associated with the hypothesized sex - specific stressors of out - group threat for males and infant distress in females?
As such, increased cortisol responses following MS induction are predicted in men exposed to out - group stressors (i.e., inflammatory speech) and in females exposed to cues of infant distress (i.e., crying).
a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z