Sentences with phrase «predicted cu»

In Barker and Salekin's study [75], experience of peer victimization at age 10 predicted CU traits at age 13 in children with a high score on a measure of irritability.
In the first study [44], temperament (fearlessness) of the biological mother predicted CU behaviour of the adopted child at 27 months, via earlier fearlessness measured at 18 months; similarly, low affiliative behaviour of biologic mothers directly predicted child CU behaviours, although without any correlation with child affiliative behaviours tested at 18 months.
Parental - reported corporal punishment and child - reported parental warmth / involvement predicted CU traits, as children with low levels of anxiety who reported low parental warmth showed increased CU features 1 year later [59].
In this respect, Waller et al. (2016), using an adoption sample as well as longitudinal measures (fearlessness was measured at 18 months, CU traits and ODD at 27 months), demonstrated that biological mother's fearlessness predicted CU traits via earlier child fearlessness.
An aspect especially worthy of note was related to predicting CU - only traits over time.
However, the association was not bidirectional and ODD at age 3 did not predict CU behaviors.
The regression model at step 1 did not significantly predict CU group membership (χ2 = 14.81, df = 12, p = 0.25).

Not exact matches

Dan Theodorescu, MD, PhD, James Costello, PhD, and CU Cancer Center colleagues show that whole - exome sequencing predicts bladder cancer sensitivity to cisplatin.
Base Price — $ 30,990 Initial Quality — 4 Predicted Reliability — 2.5 Overall Vehicle Appeal — 5 Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 13/30 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — 5 Stars * IIHS Crash Test Rating — Not a «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 36.3 cu.
Base Price — $ 31,245 Initial Quality — 2 Predicted Reliability — 3 Overall Vehicle Appeal — 3 Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 18/27 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — 5 Stars IIHS Crash Test Rating — «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 46.8 cu.
Base Price — $ 24,890 Initial Quality — 3 Predicted Reliability — 2.5 Overall Vehicle Appeal — 2 Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 16/21 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — Testing is incomplete IIHS Crash Test Rating — Not a «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 12.8 - 31.5 cu.
Base Price — $ 34,950 Initial Quality — 4 PCRs Predicted Reliability — 3 PCRs Overall Vehicle Appeal — 4 PCRs Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 17/22 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — 4 Stars IIHS Crash Test Rating — Not a «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 46.3 cu.
Base Price — $ 25,845 Initial Quality Rating — 3 Predicted Reliability — 3.5 Overall Vehicle Appeal — 3 Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 20/33 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — 5 Stars IIHS Crash Test Rating — «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 35.5 cu.
Base Price — $ 25,995 Initial Quality — 5 PCRs Predicted Reliability — 3 PCRs Overall Vehicle Appeal — 5 PCRs Fuel Economy (city / highway mpg)-- 17/29 NHTSA Crash Test Rating — 5 Stars IIHS Crash Test Rating — «Top Safety Pick» Cargo Volume Behind Second - Row Seat — 38.8 cu.
Assuming CU just hits beta but does not launch, I predict the number of successful crowd - funded MMOS, from the beginning of time through the end of the 2018, will remain zero.
Considering ice volumes over the last decade, CCSM4 AR4 is predicting a decline of some 200 cu km pa while PIOMAS models suggest something more like 600 cu km pa.
Considering ice volumes over the last decade, CCSM4 AR4 is predicting a decline of some 200 cu km pa while PIOMAS models show some 600 cu km pa.
Since its inception 8 years ago, the NCAR / CU sea ice pool has easily rivaled much more sophisticated efforts based on statistical methods and physical models to predict the September monthly mean Arctic sea ice extent (e.g. see appendix of Stroeve et al. 2014 in GRL doi: 10.1002 / 2014GL059388; Witness the Arctic article by Hamilton et al. 2014 http://www.arcus.org/witness-the-arctic/2014/2/article/21066).
Only conduct problems at time 1 and the interaction between change in Emotional Distance and CU traits were found to significantly predict the outcome variable of time 2 conduct problems (p <.05).
Age, family dysfunction change, and the interaction between CU category and family dysfunction change were found to significantly predict conduct problems at time 2 (p <.05).
Moreover, considering that various facets of parent — child interaction relate to CU traits, future longitudinal research should examine which particular dimensions (e.g., parental behavior management, relationship - based, and emotion - related) independently predict levels of childhood CU traits over time.
Results indicated significant interaction effects between ODD - related problems and CU, as well as between CU and anxiety, in predicting attentional orientation patterns for angry, fearful and happy faces.
Examination of these interactions in predicting the Peak Toward scores for angry faces revealed that, for the moderator effect of anxiety, the slope was significantly different from 0 at low levels of anxiety t (48) = − 3.14, p < 0.05, showing that for children with lower levels of anxiety, CU traits were associated with less attention orientation toward angry faces (see Figure 2).
Particularly, we showed that both attentional allocation toward and away from these facial stimuli were predicted by high CU traits and high ODD - related problems.
We predicted that such deficits would be particularly marked for sadness and fear, given previous research showing disproportionate impairments in the processing of distress cues in those with high levels of CU traits (Dadds et al. 2006; Marsh and Blair 2008; Short et al. 2016).
Waller et al. [64], found that higher levels of exposure to violence predicted both moderate and high CU traits trajectories, while no associations were found between neighbourhood disorder and CU traits trajectory.
Only among girls, CU traits also positively predicted indirect bullying.
Well designed longitudinal studies, both naturalistic and intervention, mainly in clinical samples, are needed to further delineate developmental trajectories, their predictors, and how CU traits alone or interacting with behavioural symptoms predict the outcomes.
001) and poor supervision / monitoring predicted increases in CU traits (p = 0.03) however, the inverse relations were not found.
CU traits at age 3 predicted ODD (particularly the headstrong component) at age 5, suggesting that children with cold, non-empathic, and uncaring traits are likely to show behavioral symptoms of oppositionality.
However, studies investigating the association between quality of parenting and prospective change in CU traits in preschool samples have found that multiple domains of parenting (positive parenting, parental involvement, and poor monitoring / supervision) uniquely predicted changes in CU traits [47], and that high CU traits at ages 3 — 4 were predicted by parent harshness [57].
Hierarchical regression showed that pro-social behavior was not a significant predictor of aggressive behavior when CU traits were not controlled, while CU traits predict aggressive behavior with or without pro-sociality.
It was predicted that boys with antisocial behaviour problems and high levels of CU traits would have lower levels of serum serotonin than boys with antisocial behaviour problems and low levels of CU traits.
However, CU traits predicted direct bullying positively and defending behaviors negatively.
Indeed, we acknowledge that the present findings may be considered surprising given previous work showing that empathy deficits are more pronounced in those with CD and elevated CU traits than those with lower levels of CU traits (Jones et al. 2010; Schwenck et al. 2012) and theories predicting that affective empathy deficits are uniquely related to CU traits (Blair 2005, 2013).
However, results show that high levels of CU traits predict elevated externalizing behavior problems, but only for toddlers exhibiting either high baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) or little to no RSA suppression in response to a fear stimulus.
Neither CU traits nor parasympathetic functioning at age 2 directly predicted later externalizing behaviors.
a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z